Productions need not match study items to confer a production advantage, but it helps.

dc.contributor.authorKelly, Megan O.
dc.contributor.authorLu, Xinyi
dc.contributor.authorEnsor, Tyler M.
dc.contributor.authorMacLeod, Colin M.
dc.contributor.authorRisko, Evan F.
dc.date.accessioned2025-07-02T17:47:26Z
dc.date.available2025-07-02T17:47:26Z
dc.date.issued2024
dc.descriptionThis version of the article may not completely replicate the final authoritative version published in Experimental Psychology at: https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000600 It is not the version of record and is therefore not suitable for citation.
dc.description.abstractThe production effect is the finding that, relative to silent reading, producing information at study (e.g., reading aloud) leads to a benefit in memory. In most studies of this effect, individuals are presented with a set of unique items, and they produce a subset of these items (e.g., they are presented with the to-be-remembered target item TABLE and produce “table”) such that the production is both unique and representative of the target. Across two preregistered experiments, we examined the influence of a production that is unique but that does not match the target (e.g., producing “fence” to the target TABLE, producing “car” to the target TREE, and so on). This kind of production also yielded a significant effect—the mismatching production effect—although it was smaller than the standard production effect (i.e., when productions are both unique and representative of their targets) and was detectable only when targets with "standard" productions were included in the same study phase (i.e., when the type of production was manipulated within participant). We suggest that target-production matching is an important precursor to the production effect, and that the kind of production that brings about a benefit depends on the other productions that are present.
dc.description.sponsorshipNatural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC), Alexander Graham Bell Canada Graduate Scholarship || NSERC, Discovery Grant #04091 || Province of Ontario, Early Researcher Award (#ER14-10-258) || Canada Foundation for Innovation and Ontario Research Fund, #37872 || Canada Research Chairs, #950-232147.
dc.identifier.urihttps://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000600
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/10012/21937
dc.language.isoen
dc.publisherHogrefe
dc.relation.ispartofseriesExperimental Psychology; 71(1)
dc.rightsAttribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 Internationalen
dc.rights.urihttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/
dc.titleProductions need not match study items to confer a production advantage, but it helps.
dc.typeArticle
dcterms.bibliographicCitationKelly, M. O., Lu, X., Ensor, T. M., MacLeod, C. M., & Risko, E. F. (2024). Productions need not match study items to confer a production advantage, but it helps. Experimental Psychology, 71(1), 2–13. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000600
uws.contributor.affiliation1Faculty of Arts
uws.contributor.affiliation2Psychology
uws.peerReviewStatusReviewed
uws.scholarLevelFaculty
uws.typeOfResourceTexten

Files

Original bundle

Now showing 1 - 1 of 1
No Thumbnail Available
Name:
Kellyetal (2024) productions need not match study items to confer a production advantage, but it helps.pdf
Size:
534.89 KB
Format:
Adobe Portable Document Format

License bundle

Now showing 1 - 1 of 1
No Thumbnail Available
Name:
license.txt
Size:
4.47 KB
Format:
Item-specific license agreed upon to submission
Description: