Productions need not match study items to confer a production advantage, but it helps.
dc.contributor.author | Kelly, Megan O. | |
dc.contributor.author | Lu, Xinyi | |
dc.contributor.author | Ensor, Tyler M. | |
dc.contributor.author | MacLeod, Colin M. | |
dc.contributor.author | Risko, Evan F. | |
dc.date.accessioned | 2025-07-02T17:47:26Z | |
dc.date.available | 2025-07-02T17:47:26Z | |
dc.date.issued | 2024 | |
dc.description | This version of the article may not completely replicate the final authoritative version published in Experimental Psychology at: https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000600 It is not the version of record and is therefore not suitable for citation. | |
dc.description.abstract | The production effect is the finding that, relative to silent reading, producing information at study (e.g., reading aloud) leads to a benefit in memory. In most studies of this effect, individuals are presented with a set of unique items, and they produce a subset of these items (e.g., they are presented with the to-be-remembered target item TABLE and produce “table”) such that the production is both unique and representative of the target. Across two preregistered experiments, we examined the influence of a production that is unique but that does not match the target (e.g., producing “fence” to the target TABLE, producing “car” to the target TREE, and so on). This kind of production also yielded a significant effect—the mismatching production effect—although it was smaller than the standard production effect (i.e., when productions are both unique and representative of their targets) and was detectable only when targets with "standard" productions were included in the same study phase (i.e., when the type of production was manipulated within participant). We suggest that target-production matching is an important precursor to the production effect, and that the kind of production that brings about a benefit depends on the other productions that are present. | |
dc.description.sponsorship | Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC), Alexander Graham Bell Canada Graduate Scholarship || NSERC, Discovery Grant #04091 || Province of Ontario, Early Researcher Award (#ER14-10-258) || Canada Foundation for Innovation and Ontario Research Fund, #37872 || Canada Research Chairs, #950-232147. | |
dc.identifier.uri | https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000600 | |
dc.identifier.uri | https://hdl.handle.net/10012/21937 | |
dc.language.iso | en | |
dc.publisher | Hogrefe | |
dc.relation.ispartofseries | Experimental Psychology; 71(1) | |
dc.rights | Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International | en |
dc.rights.uri | http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ | |
dc.title | Productions need not match study items to confer a production advantage, but it helps. | |
dc.type | Article | |
dcterms.bibliographicCitation | Kelly, M. O., Lu, X., Ensor, T. M., MacLeod, C. M., & Risko, E. F. (2024). Productions need not match study items to confer a production advantage, but it helps. Experimental Psychology, 71(1), 2–13. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000600 | |
uws.contributor.affiliation1 | Faculty of Arts | |
uws.contributor.affiliation2 | Psychology | |
uws.peerReviewStatus | Reviewed | |
uws.scholarLevel | Faculty | |
uws.typeOfResource | Text | en |
Files
Original bundle
1 - 1 of 1
No Thumbnail Available
- Name:
- Kellyetal (2024) productions need not match study items to confer a production advantage, but it helps.pdf
- Size:
- 534.89 KB
- Format:
- Adobe Portable Document Format
License bundle
1 - 1 of 1
No Thumbnail Available
- Name:
- license.txt
- Size:
- 4.47 KB
- Format:
- Item-specific license agreed upon to submission
- Description: