Show simple item record

dc.contributor.authorPhan, Vincent
dc.contributor.authorNishioka, Midori
dc.contributor.authorBeck, James W.
dc.contributor.authorScholer, Abigail A.
dc.date.accessioned2023-05-02 16:57:49 (GMT)
dc.date.available2023-05-02 16:57:49 (GMT)
dc.date.issued2023-04
dc.identifier.urihttps://doi.org/10.1037/apl0001037
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/10012/19390
dc.description©American Psychological Association, 2023. This paper is not the copy of record and may not exactly replicate the authoritative document published in the APA journal. Please do not copy or cite without author's permission. The final article is available, upon publication, at: https://doi.org/10.1037/apl0001037en
dc.description.abstractEmployees often have a great deal of work to accomplish within stringent deadlines. Therefore, employees may engage in shortcut behaviors, which involve eschewing standard procedures during goal pursuit to save time. However, shortcuts can lead to negative consequences such as poor-quality work, accidents, and even large-scale disasters. Despite these implications, few studies have investigated the antecedents of shortcut behaviors. In this research, we propose that employees engage in shortcut behaviors to regulate their velocity (i.e., rate of progress). Specifically, we predict that when individuals experience slower-than-referent velocity, they will (1) believe that the goal is unlikely to be met via standard procedures (2) and experience feelings of frustration. In turn, we expect these psychological states to be related to the perceived utility of shortcuts, especially when shortcuts are perceived as viable means to achieve the goal. Finally, we predict that the perceived utility of shortcuts will be positively related to actual shortcut behaviors. We tested these predictions using a laboratory experiment in which we manipulated velocity and unobtrusively observed shortcuts (Study 1, N = 147), as well as a daily diary study in which employees reported their velocity and shortcut behaviors over five consecutive workdays (Study 2, N = 395). Both studies provided support for our predictions. In sum, this research provides evidence to suggest that the experience of slow progress can lead to shortcuts not only by casting doubt on employees’ perceived likelihood of meeting the goal, but also by producing feelings of frustration.en
dc.description.sponsorshipSocial Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (SSHRC), Insight Grant 435-2018-0488en
dc.language.isoenen
dc.publisherAmerican Psychological Associationen
dc.relation.ispartofseriesJournal of Applied Psychology;
dc.subjectshortcut behaviorsen
dc.subjectvelocityen
dc.subjectexpectancyen
dc.subjectfrustrationen
dc.subjectutilityen
dc.titleGoal Progress Velocity as a Determinant of Shortcut Behaviorsen
dc.typeArticleen
dcterms.bibliographicCitationPhan, V., Nishioka, M., Beck, J. W., & Scholer, A. A. (2023). Goal progress velocity as a determinant of shortcut behaviors.Journal of Applied Psychology, 108(4), 553–570. https://doi.org/10.1037/apl0001037en
uws.contributor.affiliation1Faculty of Artsen
uws.contributor.affiliation2Psychologyen
uws.typeOfResourceTexten
uws.peerReviewStatusRevieweden
uws.scholarLevelFacultyen
uws.scholarLevelGraduateen


Files in this item

Thumbnail

This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record


UWSpace

University of Waterloo Library
200 University Avenue West
Waterloo, Ontario, Canada N2L 3G1
519 888 4883

All items in UWSpace are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved.

DSpace software

Service outages